Survey
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
* Your assessment is very important for improving the workof artificial intelligence, which forms the content of this project
Spaced Retrieval (SR) Memory Enhancement in Alzheimer’s Disease The OVERVIEW This Magic Moment Dr. Cameron Camp has written extensively about the benefits of using spaced – retrieval (SR) techniques with those who suffer from neurological disorders such as dementia. His research provides compelling evidence of the “magic” that occurs when the techniques are applied. The magic began in the fall of 1986. “I was working in an adult day health care centre with two student research assistants. We had been gathering initial data on a memory battery for everyday memory problems. We were especially interested in observing how individuals diagnosed as having . . . Alzheimer’s . . . would perform on the various subtests of the battery. After the testing session, the client was asked to recall the names of each of the two students in turn. She could not do so. After each recall failure, she was provided with the correct name. She was then asked to name the psychologist. Again she failed and was provided the correct name. A few seconds later, she was asked to name the psychologist, and did so. She was then asked to name each student in turn, and failed. She was then asked to name the psychologist, and did so. Next, she was asked to name each of the students in turn. This was repeated twice (three pairs, six recall attempts). She failed on each recall attempt of a student’s name. She then was asked to name the psychologist, and did so. Thus the paradoxical result was that the seldompresented psychologist’s name was remembered best. On the way out, the student asked the client how she had been able to remember the psychologist’s name. The client responded that she did not know. . . (The) students asked the psychologist what he had used to let the woman learn his name. His response was ‘magic,’ and he then relayed the history of that seemingly ‘magical’ intervention to his students” (Camp, 1989, page 212). What is the incantation that creates such magic? The answer comes from the magical spells that were originally cast by Landauer and Bjork (1978). They began with a name-learning technique called “spaced-retrieval”, where names of faces were recalled at increasingly longer intervals over a fixed period of time (Camp, 1989). The questions they wanted to answer were, “Could face-name associations be retained over time (long-term retention)?” and “How does the timing of the information affect retention?” 1 Unlocking the Mystery! Cameron, This confuses me a bit. Repetition works with studying, etc. Some remember things better than others. Is the SR technique making a point mostly for those with memory problems or for everyone? Is memory enhancement the result of practice (repetition)? OR Is memory enhancement a result of something else? For example, increasingly longer, strategically timed intervals between testing? Landauer and Bjork (1978) concluded that practice or repetition alone was not the key to unlocking the mystery. They found that if intervals between recalls are consistently and systematically expanded, retrieval of information was more likely to be successful. Therefore, spaced-retrieval contributes to memory enhancement (or in other words, successful attempts to recall the targeted information is a result of the spacing between repetitions). Landmark Finding Bjork (1979) discovered that spaced-retrieval may outweigh the successes of other memory training protocols because it requires little cognitive effort. Since people who suffer from neurological impairments find it difficult, to impossible, to engage in conscious cognitive manipulations, organizational strategies and abstract thought, a simple memory enhancing strategy is essential. The spacedretrieval technique focuses on strengthening capabilities rather than frustrating the already impaired areas of function. “Explicit memory and effortful information processing are devastated in persons with dementia, and attempts to use conscious processing to store and retrieve information generally fail in this population.” Camp & Foss (1997), pg 312 2 OUR OBJECTIVE: To help individuals with memory problems, such as those who bear the burden associated with the devastating memory decline experienced by those with dementia. The objective then is to teach the individual who suffers from memory loss, as well as those involved with their care, evidence-based strategies that will improve their ability to carry out daily activities. (This can range from something as simple as remembering the name of their spouse to more complex tasks such as paying bills or being taught to remember to grab the arms of a chair before sitting.) Implicit memory contributes to the success of spacedretrieval techniques. Cherry, et al. (1999) HOW DO WE ACHIEVE THIS DAUNTING TASK WHEN THE INDIVIDUAL SUFFERS FROM MEMORY DECLINE? Employ evidence-based strategies that are known to assist with learning (or relearning). The magical solution: Spaced-Retrieval techniques for dementia. Examples of information/tasks that can be learned: Memory training to assist with ordinary living. Names of family, friends and staff. Finding destinations (washroom, bedroom, kitchen). Helping the individual to remember when the next meal is and what time of the day it is (information finding). Finding items such as clothing, memory books, purse or watch. Remembering his/her own name. Research evidence suggests that individuals with Dementia of the Alzheimers Type (DAT) “show substantial decrements in the active processing of new information” (Camp, xxxx , page 156 article is called Memory interventions in AD populations). The root of the problem lies with severely limited capacity to actively process new information into the memory storage system. Thus, the information is collected, but not filed for future utilization. This is comparable 3 to placing a file in a locked filing cabinet. When you return to pull the file, you realize you have lost the key. Your access to the file is therefore blocked. You don’t have access to the information in the file, just as the person with DAT does not have access to the memory he/she is searching for. If you find another way to get into the cabinet, or cut another key to provide access into the cabinet, you will have success at finding the information you are looking for. Spacedretrieval provides the method of accessing important information over time, thus providing you with a different means of accessing the much sought after information. THE PROBLEM Information comes in. Information is Stored. THE SOLUTION Find a way to retrieve information successfully. (Hint: techniques.) Retrieval of information problematic. Spaced-Retrieval The Technique As noted previously, the intent of Spaced-retrieval is Spaced-Retrieval to help a person with dementia, or sundry other A memory intervention, cognitive impairments that impact memory (e.g. – effective for those with Parkinson’s, amnesia), recall information over dementia, that focuses on correct recall of increasingly longer periods of time. The objective is to information over help the individual access important information, thus increasing intervals of enhancing their abilities to deal with everyday time. Retention of information can last activities of living. While these methods are indeed days, weeks, and even valuable for the person with dementia, they also months (Camp, 2000). address some of the behaviours that are regarded as problematic for family, friends and caregivers (such as repetitive questioning, disorientation, aggressiveness and wandering). 4 Spaced-retrieval successfully links research from the neurosciences with a real world setting. The techniques can be learned and adopted by both professionals and family caregivers (Camp, et al., 1996). Case Situation A caregiver relayed the following example to Dr. Cameron Camp after she had attended an educational session describing Spaced-retrieval: “I drive my mother to the Azlheimer’s centre each weekday. Since we live on the north shore . . . and the centre is on the south shore, this is a long drive . . . . All the way to the centre my mom would keep asking ‘Where are we going?’ I’d tell her ‘We’re going to the school’ (that was the name the family used to refer to the centre). Then she’d ask again, and again, and again. It used to drive me crazy. Well, after your talk I decided to try the thing you were talking about. So the next day when we got into the car I said, ‘Mom, we’re going to school now’. Then I waited a few seconds and asked her, ‘Where are we going?’ She looked at me like I was a little crazy and said, ‘We’re going to school, like you said”. That was the last I brought up the subject, but you know, she never asked me that question the whole trip. Now I ask her when we get in the car, she answers, and that’s that. I just thought you’d like to know that is actually works’ (Camp, Foss, O’Hanlon and Stevens, 1995, page 193).” If you work with cognitively impaired older adults you know how frustrating information seeking/repetitive questioning can be for both the caregiver and the person who suffers from memory loss. Those who are severely memory impaired are more dramatically affected, and those providing care often become impatient and, at times, ready to avoid contact at all costs. It is not surprising that Coy, Kinney, Cavanaugh and Dunn (1992) discovered that memory problems are the most frequently-cited source of irritation (or hassles) reported by caregivers. When they do not know how to be effective at helping, negative feelings proliferate. These researchers found that positive feelings are generated when caregivers learn how to do something “proactive with and for a memory-impaired person and the feeling of purpose and involvement the impaired person experiences from engaging in an effortful cooperative task make the effort worthwhile in and of itself’ (page 18) [in Camp, et al., 1996). “Repetition alone is not enough to produce a memorial benefit for older adults with probable AD. Rather, it seems that spaced-retrieval training is necessary in order to observe a memorial benefi.” Cherry and SimmonsD’Gerolamo (1999), page 48 5 Spaced-Retrieval: A Simple Overview of the Techniques in Action Spaced-Retrieval techniques are evidence-based approaches that have proven to effectively address many of the issues that arise as a result of memory loss. The paragraphs below provide an overview of the techniques. More complete details are provided in the chapter that follows. Dr. Cameron Camp’s approach with those who have been diagnosed with dementia has focused on the following technique: 1. providing the individual with specific information to remember, 2. then asking the individual to immediately recall the information, 3. then asking the question using a strategic plan of spaced intervals (increasing the intervals at each successful interval, returning to the previous interval when not successful) until retention over time has been accomplished (e.g. – weeks to months) (Cherry, Simmon and Camp, 1999). Step 1: Determine Learning Activity (Target information) You will need to decide what information you want the participant in the Spaced-retrieval session to learn. This can be referred to as the learning activity or the “target information”. In the example below, Mrs. Smith is being asked to recall the name of her caregiver. This was regarded as the most bothersome problem for Mrs. Smith. She always seemed to be distressed about who was going to be looking after her. However, if she had engaged in repetitive questioning with a different type of question (e.g. – “Where is my room?”, “When am I going to eat?”, “Where are my clothes?”) one of these questions could have been chosen as your target. Targets can include: Associations between names and faces (family, staff and/or friends). Objects and their locations. Procedural tasks/directions such as where to look for information about when the next meal will be served or to remember to grab the arms of a chair before sitting down. 6 Begin by telling the person that he/she will be working with you to learn or remember things. Tell the person your name. In this case, begin by telling her the name of her caregiver, then proceed by asking the target question (in this case you would be asking for her caregiver’s name) (the answer may be on a cue card for her to read or on a card with her caregiver’s picture, with her name typed beside/below the picture. During the first sessions with Mrs. Smith the cue card can be used as a memory aid). Let’s practice. What is your caregiver’s name? Begin by telling her, “Your caregiver’s name is Joan”. Then ask, “What is your caregiver’s name?” You can then prompt her to turn the cue card over and read the information. My caregiver’s name is Joan. Now ask immediately This is the cue card. Step 2 It is quite appropriate to talk about other things during the waiting interval. If you “chat” it is important to use a timing device that helps you stick to your interval schedule. If the recall attempt was successful (she told you her caregiver’s name is Joan) the next time interval time will be expanded systematically. You could space the intervals by 5, 10, 20, 40 or 60 seconds. So the next time you ask the question, “What is your caregiver’s name?” could be 10 seconds later (as presented in the example below). If the space of time you selected was successful, then you can continue to increase the interval each time the increased interval is successful. If the attempt to recall the information fails, and she cannot remember the name of her caregiver, you then return to the last successful interval time (e.g. – if you were at 40, had moved to 60 but 60 was not successful, you provide the information that is targeted and return to the 40 second interval). 7 Spaced-Retrieval Example Now ask 10 seconds later What is your caregiver’s name? My caregiver’s name if Joan. This correct response is referred to as an errorless trial. Now ask 20 seconds later What is your caregiver’s name? My caregiver’s name if Joan. Repeat again at 40 seconds, 60 seconds, etc. If the attempt is not successful remind her that her caregiver’s name is Joan and go back to the last successful time interval. Once the correct answer is provided, you can then start to increase the interval times again. The results should be recorded, and the last successful time (e.g. – 60 seconds) will be used as the starting point in Session 2. The training session typically lasts for 30 minutes (Camp, 1989; McKitrick and Camp, 1989). Step 3 One week later, referred to as the second training session, the individual will be asked the targeted information from the previous session. In this case, the question is, “What is your caregiver’s name?” The individual’s ability to retain the answer from the previous session is tested the first time the question is asked in the second session. If the participant cannot remember the answer, you begin by providing the correct answer and then the first interval between questions will be the last successful timing that was recorded in the previous session. Record keeping is therefore essential. 8 If you begin by increasing the interval time based on the length of spaced intervals used in the first session and the correct response is provided, you do not need to continue the session. Retention has been successful and has exceeded the last interval used in the previous session. The objective is to move towards long-term retention (although permanent retention may not always transpire since neurological damage will continue as the dementia progresses). Expanding the training Once the recall target has been reached (e.g. – retention has been successful for as long as 5 weeks), you can begin with a new objective. For example, you may want to incorporate something more complicated, such as learning to look for the answer on a cue card. Case Example A very impaired gentleman was taught to read his calendar. He required daily orienting information about his day/activities. His task for this particular day was to water the plants. He returned to tell his wife that it had been raining and was there something else he could do? This proved that he understood what he had to do – a step beyond the ability to just repeating the words. Camp, et al. (1996) You could focus on providing an answer to a repetitive question (one that may be driving staff crazy) or with a functional activity related to safety (such as take a sip of your drink before a bite of solid food), where to look to find out what is scheduled for today, where to find information about today (the reality orientation board) or something else that has been identified as important for this person to remember. When you ask the target question, you will direct this person to look at the card and read the answer to you. The target could therefore be to look at the card, a page or information board to find details about daily activities and events. You will set up a timed interval schedule just as you did in the case above. Adding variety to the training Spaced-retrieval techniques can be used in conjunction with other activities, as long as the interval schedule is maintained throughout the session. The memory training can take place while you play cards, engage in conversation, cut and style the person’s hair, look through photograph albums, participate in arts and crafts activities or listen to music. 9 SpacedRetrieval With The Cognitively Impaired New Learning + Long-term retention of simple associations (simple, everyday objects are best) Spaced-retrieval has been studied over time using different approaches and testing different variables. Cherry, Simmons and Camp (1999) tested free recall as a measure of explicit memory, but found that the DAT subjects were unable to free-recall the target objects. They concluded that free recall is perhaps too demanding for those with mild/moderate DAT. This same study also assessed explicit memory from the perspective of object recognition. This proved to be more successful, thus leading to the conclusion that recognition abilities are not affected. Thus, ability to remember previously acquired information is spared in dementia. These tasks were completed using Spaced-retrieval techniques. The research concluded that spaced-retrieval techniques do indeed improve retention in older adults with probable DAT. In fact, they state “that Spacedretrieval effects are robust and generalize across settings, materials, and procedures (Camp, 1989; McKitrick, et al., 1992). In addition, we found modest evidence to suggest that implicit memory contributes to spaced-retrieval effects. . . and the maintenance of spaced-retrieval effects over time” (Cherry, Simmons, and Camp, 1999, page 172). 10 Spaced-Retrieval: The Process Record results Schedule next training session (e.g. – a week later). Determine timing of intervals for next session. Assess and Screen to determine eligibility. Develop a recording sheet for the participant. This will be used at each session. Identify the information that you will target for the SR sessions (e.g. - the name of a staff member, the person’s room number, where their clothes are kept, using daily calendar, etc.) PLAN Question & Interval Schedule Ask the designated question. (This is often referred to as the target information.) Wait for the response. If correct, follow the predetermined schedule of intervals. If response incorrect, return to last successful time. Combine the spaced-retrieval training with activities such as playing cards, chatting or other social activities. If the expanded interval is successful at the start of the next session there no need to hold the session. If not successful, return to last timing of successful retention from last session. Continue for pre-determined number of sessions or until information has been retained. Introduce new targeted information if you wish the person to acquire other information (e.g. – begin by learning the name of a staff member then focus on a question that is asked repeatedly, such as, “Where is my room?” Once the information has been retained, continue to test over time. TIME Interval tailored to meet client abilities 30 minutes per session RECORD Plan for retention Length of the Session As stated above, the session should last for about 30 minutes. You will find that if you try to lengthen the session the person with DAT will become fatigued, thus affecting their performance as the time extends beyond the optimal period (Cherry, Simmons & Camp, 1999). Dr. Cameron Camp usually offers a training session to a person with DAT once a week, but, depending on circumstances, this may vary. For example, if performance is affected because one week is too long for retention to be successful, the period of time between sessions may need to be shortened. 11 Spaced-retrieval techniques have proven to contribute to retention of the information over time. Cherry, et al. (1999) found that participants performed progressively better across training session, but also continued the task for longer periods of time across sessions and had fewer failures over time (more failures on the first session than the second, fewer on the second and the fewer again on the third). The more cognitively impaired the individual is, the shorter the time interval will be when you begin. You can build to the longer intervals, but don’t begin with the longer interval. As discussed earlier, implicit memory processes are believed to be spared in DAT until the later stages (Camp, et al., 1993; Squire, 1994)). The person with DAT will therefore not seem to be aware of the learning that has taken place. Spaced-Retrieval: Shaping Memory Spaced-retrieval is a form of shaping. Shaping has its origins in behaviour modification, where shaping is described as “the reinforcement of successive approximations to a desired behaviour” (Camp & Mattern, 1999, page 281). The reinforcement is intrinsic for older adults who are coping with memory loss. Each time they successfully recall the information they are being trained to remember, they are intrinsically positively reinforced for their effort. The reinforcement component of the definition is therefore attended to without your conscious involvement. The expansion schedule, however, is up to you to establish. When an expansion schedule (increasing the time between intervals across time) is put into place, it is a form of shaping (Bjork, 1988; Landauer & Bjork, 1978; Camp, 1999). The term shaping thus means that you are engaging in an activity, where positive reinforcement is involved, that will change behaviour. When working with older adults who are experiencing increasing problems with memory loss, the shaping is focused on strengthening this person’s ability to remember information. Spaced-Retrieval Other words that describe SR: = Memory training = Memory therapy = Learning/ relearning = Shaping memory = Memory intervention = Memory therapy The primary objective is for the person with DAT to retain targeted information over time (e.g. – weeks or years) (Camp, 1999). The time between intervals is determined according to the performance of the participant. A “spaced-retrieval training procedure is a shaping paradigm” (Camp, 1999, page 165). A sample schedule is presented below. 12 Spaced-Retrieval: Shaping Memory Sample graph with time intervals by learning trial Time in seconds seseconds 140 120 100 80 Series1 60 Correct Response 40 20 Incorrect Response 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Trial Recording Results Dr. Cameron Camp has found that computer technology helps to keep careful track of the expansion schedules. In one study, (Clinical Gerontologist), he used a small digital countdown timer and monitored the intervals “with the aid of a simple chart with a sliding scale that could be adjusted for each successive interval.” (ibid page 60 ask Cameron to explain – what is this piece of equipment??) Who can be trained to use Spaced-retrieval Techniques? What disciplines use SR Techniques? o Occupational Therapy o Physiotherapy o Speech Language Specialists o Music & Art Therapists Anyone who is properly trained to use spaced-retrieval methodology can use these techniques successfully. Professionals (including occupational therapists, speech-language therapists, life enrichment coordinators, recreation therapists, social workers, nurses and case-managers), para-professionals (social support workers, aides, and other caregivers), volunteers, family, caregivers and friends will find that this form of intervention can prove to deliver rewarding results. It should be noted that caregivers might benefit from simplified techniques to make their learning and application of the procedures easier (McKitrick & Camp, in Clinical Gerontologist year?) - CAMERON I NEED INFO 13 Who can benefit from this form of memory enhancement? Spaced-retrieval can be used as a therapeutic technique with individuals who have been diagnosed with memory impairment, including dementia of the Alzheimer’s Type (DAT) (Camp, et al., 1996); dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease (Hayden & Camp, 1995); dementia associated with Korsakoff’s Syndrome (Camp & Schaller, 1989); vascular and mixed dementia (Abrahams & Camp, 1993, Bird, Alexopouls & Ademowitz, 1995); post-anoxia dementia (Bird, et al., 1995) and cerebrovascular accident (Brush, et al., 1998). For example, an individual with Parkinson’s Disease learned new motor skills as a result of the Spaced-retrieval training, a task that had otherwise seemed to be close to impossible (Brush, et al., 1989). Little cognitive effort is required with Spaced-retrieval interventions and, as was discussed in the chapter on memory, the person with dementia (or other forms of memory impairment) who engages in the training does not remember learning the information. Who Benefits? Individuals who suffer from memory loss (e.g. – DAT, Parkinson’s Disease, Vascular Dementia) Staff All those who provide care to someone who is challenged by memory loss Family & Friends Volunteers Where can Spaced-Retrieval Techniques be used? As long as the people using the techniques are properly trained, these techniques can be used anywhere. In fact, a trained family member could engage in Spaced-retrieval memory intervention while going for a car ride or during meal time, by incorporating the training into the drive or dinner time conversation. These techniques can therefore be used in the community as well as in long-term care facilities. Making Connections Between Memory and Your Approach to Intervention FOCUS ON SPARED CAPACITY An essential component of a successful memory intervention is that of focusing on the capabilities (e.g. – calling on memory function that is spared in dementia, for instance ability to perform motor tasks such as watering plants, folding laundry or washing dishes; reading and even following the instructions on printed cue cards; counting forward; doing things that have been practiced to the point of being habits [such as brushing teeth and washing]; and sorting) rather than deficits (e.g. – requiring the individual to concentrate; learning/relearning using traditional rehearsal methods; or calling on explicit 14 memory that requires knowledge, particularly if has been acquired recently, thus depending on the impaired short-term memory structures). Since implicit memory involves the unconscious retrieval of information, thus affording the individual the luxury of retrieving information without cognitive effort, it is important to capitalize on the use of implicit memory when designing interventions for dementia. Since the explicit memory structures become increasingly devastated with dementia, it is important to remember that those who are experiencing progressive memory loss simply can’t remember concepts and knowledge as brain damage continues! Therefore, when the person needs to think about what he/she wants to remember, there is effort required, with no guarantee that the information will be retrieved. Some long-term memories remain relatively intact, but day-to-day activities of living often don’t depend on these biographical memories (e.g. – memories about life at home, parents, loved ones, jobs, home environment or accomplishments, such as awards won or important objectives that were reached.) Oh How Interesting and Yet So Vitally Important to Know When implicit/procedural memory is strengthened the individual in early to mid stages of dementia can be taught to remember, without remembering he/she just learned to remember. Reinforcing Memory Concepts As mentioned in the chapter on memory, Dr. Cameron Camp devised a model that is not only memorable, but functional. If you remember the song (calling, of course, on your explicit memory bank) “Old McDonald Had a Farm”, you will not only have this song in your head for the remainder of the day, but you can use this song to remember the framework for dementia memory interventions. As mentioned earlier, Dr. Camp’s model is called the E-I-E-I-O model. This model clearly illustrates which type of memory is responsible for what, and then identifies the type of intervention (external – where you manipulate the environment, and internal – where you focus on internal manipulations related to remembering). The matrix chart presented below is also presented in the chapter on Memory but is repeated here because of its general importance, and more specifically to reinforce its importance in relation 15 to spaced-retrieval techniques. Be sure to return to this matrix as you consider the various types of interventions presented in this book, as well as the ones you develop in your own practice. The E-I-E-I-O model provides examples of the types of intervention that you can use (external versus internal) by type of memory (explicit versus implicit). The type of intervention might include both internal and external interventions. For example, an internal intervention such as spaced-retrieval might also involve the use of external environmental manipulations such as written cue cards used for priming with the purpose of addressing a particular problem behaviour (e.g. – remembering caregiver’s name). Old McDonald’s Farm: The E-I-E-I-O Memory Intervention Model Camp, et al. (1993) developed a functional classification system that clearly illustrates the associations between type of memory (Explicit and Implicit) and intended storage location of information or cue (External or Internal). This truly serves as a cognitive/memory prosthetic for those who are learning about memory interventions! Explicit Memory (Conscious recollection of past experiences – recall & recognition of information) External cues (referred to as external storage) are used to trigger conscious remembering. E xternal Storage (In the physical environment) Information available externally (e.g. – signs, activity schedule). Implicit Memory MEMORY SYSTEM (small “s” system) STORAGE SYSTEM (Unconscious storage & retrieval of memory – priming, motor skills & habits) Initially the information is available externally, and then stored internally. I nternal Storage (Within a Person’s Memory) Verbal priming – exposure to information - taps into internal storage. Oh! When individual retrieves target information, they generate an “Oh” (I’m impressed/surprised) reaction. 16 Spaced-retrieval: “(In) brief, it involves giving individuals practice at successfully remembering information at increasingly longer time intervals. It has been described as an example of priming, or improving facility for detecting or processing stimuli on the basis of recent experience, which may include learning of new information (Squire, 1994) . . . ‘Priming can be thought of as a form of implicit memory recollection of a previous encounter with a stimulus (page 234)’”. Camp & Foss (1997), page 318 Outcomes Dr. Camp has repeatedly found that Spaced-retrieval techniques/training successfully contributes to long-term retention of new information. Research on memory posits that spared capacity of implicit memory is responsible for the success of the Spaced-retrieval techniques. This research affords increasing evidence to support that some memory systems continue to function in DAT. Not only does the intervention help the person to remember, the participants enjoy the session. In fact, those who have participated in Spaced-retrieval training have expressed how much they enjoy being involved and look forward to their next session. Dr. Cameron Camp has called this “effortless learning” because participants have told him that he makes it easy for them to remember. In fact, one participant told him, “Don’t worry, I’m not going to forget”. Moreover, it is interesting to note that one individual commented on how much she enjoyed being involved in these sessions because they made her feel “smarter” (Camp and Stevens, 1990). When one is coping with so many losses and deficits, this is surely a marvelous outcome. Next steps – external aids If these can be used as priming tools, will assist with ADL’s 17 18